About the Wild Life in Our Homes (at least the single-celled kind)

The initial paper from the Wild Life in Our Homes study by Dunn et al. found a correlation between the presence of dogs and specific bacterial communities on pillowcases and TV screens.
The initial paper from the Wild Life in Our Homes study by Dunn et al. found a correlation between the presence of dogs and specific bacterial communities on pillowcases and TV screens.

Back in the fall, I received a sampling kit, an Informed Consent form and instructions for collecting samples for the Wild Life In Our Homes citizen science project. I carefully swabbed the requested surfaces: exterior and interior door trim, kitchen counter tops, pillowcases, etc., and sent my samples in. I later received confirmation that my samples had been received and again later confirmation that they were being analyzed.

The first paper from this project has been published by Dunn et al. in PLOS ONE (Home Life: Factors Structuring the Bacterial Diversity Found within and between Homes). This initial report covers the first 40 homes sampled, all from the Raleigh-Durham, NC, USA area. Volunteers sampled their homes in the Fall of 2011, collecting specimens from nine areas: cutting boards, kitchen counters, refrigerator, toilet seat, pillowcase, door handle, TV screen, and interior and exterior door trim. The scientists used direct PCR and high-throughput sequencing to sequence the bacterial 16S rRNA gene from the submitted samples. By doing this they were able to estimate the diversity within each sample—they did not distinguish between live and dead organisms, and they did not sequence anything other than the bacterial 16SrRNA, so this study is limited to bacteria. Continue reading “About the Wild Life in Our Homes (at least the single-celled kind)”

From Gray Wolf to Bulldog: Changes to the Dog Brain as Humans Reshape it’s Head

Gray wolf photo.
Ancestor of all dogs, the gray wolf.

We humans are masters of reinvention. And we love our dogs. Over the past 12,000 years we’ve mixed that love of innovation and love of dogs, to create a species with more physical variation than any other species on Earth.

English Bulldog image.
A shorter-headed dog, the English Bulldog.

Much of this innovation has served us well. Domesticated dogs have protected our livestock and our homes, helped us hunt and retrieve game, and carried our belongings. And come the cold, snowy weather season, or a pesky avalanche, it’s nice to know that a Saint Bernard dog may be nearby with a cask of brandy on it’s collar. Seriously, rescue dogs have aided many a lost, stranded or endangered person.
St. Bernard dog with cask.

Dog breeding has served to enhance the good traits as well as to remove from a dog breed, certain bad genetic components, such as predisposition to genetically-linked diseases, as well as traits such as hip dysplasia, and eye or cardiac anomalies.

Briard dog photo.
The eyes of this Briard dog are hard to see for all the hair.

As for the variations in type, dog breeding has developed such variety as a nearly hairless dog, dogs with highly shortened faces, and dogs with so much hair that their faces are difficult to distinguish.

Chinese Crested dog.
Not so much hair, on the Chinese Crested Dog.

The extremes affect height, weight, hair (or lack of  hair, as is the case of the Chinese Crested dog). For that matter, many dogs have a haircoat that is shed, similar to wolves, coyotes and fox, while others (poodles and many terriors) have hair, that is not shed, but rather must be trimmed.

If you know a greyhound or collie dog, you know the long–headed  feature, similar to that of the original dog ancestor, the gray wolf. This morphological feature is described as dolichocephalic (long-skulled). On the other hand, if you happen to be supporting the habits of, say a Neopolitan Mastiff or a Pug dog, that makes you in the caregiver of a brachiocephalic canine, that is a short-skulled dog. Such dog breeds show a greater deviation from the ancestral gray wolf than a long-skulled dog. And as a recent study shows, more than just the head shape of brachiocephalic dogs has changed.

A July 2010 report in PLos ONE (Roberts, T., et al.; 1), noted that the extreme diversity in shape and size found in domestic dogs extends to the canine skull.

The authors set out to determine if these apparent physical features, skull shape differences, translated to differences in brain structure.

Cephalic index (CI) of the study dogs was calculated from digital measurements of length and width of the skull. As a starting point, the authors used the the  gray wolf, for which various reports listed a cephalic index of 50.9 to 51.9.
Eleven dogs,  representing various breeds and crossbreeds were used. The dogs were from shelters, and had been recently euthanized, for reasons other than the purpose of this study.  An additional two dogs used were English Springer Spaniels, who were also not euthanized for this study, rather were measured while under sedation.

The cephalic indices reported ranged from 42.2 in a greyhound to 87.2 in a shih tzu cross, the higher number indicating a shorter skull shape. The authors noted that body size and weight were closely related to CI in the domestic dogs studied.

The rather dramatic sagittal scans in the report, show a comparison of a brachiocephalic and dolichocephalic dogs with the brain outline and center of mass marked in red, and the olfactory bulb in yellow. The authors (in Figure 2) showed the two extremes of very short and very long-skulled dogs, with the brain positional differences no less striking.

The olfactory bulb was found to be positioned differently in dogs with a shortened skull. These dogs showed a definite ventral or forward (looking at a cross-section from the front of the nose to the back of the skull) shift of the brain, as well as of the olfactory bulb. Thus the authors note a strong correlation between a high CI and both central axis rotation ventrally, as well as a ventral rotation of the olfactory bulb.

This dog owner would be very interested in whether shorter–skulled dogs have behavorial changes that could be connected to their breeding. Let’s face it, the next time our dogs lunge at the dog walking by, it would help to be able to shrug and call out, “Sorry, short head”. Or, “He can’t help it, he was bred that way.”

Sadly, that’s not how it works. Once assuming ownership of a certain type of dog, the onus is on the owner to teach the dog what is and is not acceptable behavior.

But  if future studies may show a connection between cephalic index of particular dog breeds and behavioral characteristics, that information might be useful in choosing a dog. Say you wanted a dog for protection and it had been proven that a pug’s head shape was not ideal for guard dog-type behavior. You would instead choose a Collie. Umm… make that a Doberman Pinscher.  Of course Italian Greyhounds are also long-headed dogs, although known more for speed of flight, than for protective tendencies.

Reference
ResearchBlogging.org
1. Roberts T, McGreevy P, & Valenzuela M (2010). Human induced rotation and reorganization of the brain of domestic dogs. PloS one, 5 (7) PMID: 20668685